Sexual selection is a mechanism of evolution in which members of one sex mate choice of the other sex to mating with (intersexual selection), and compete with members of the same sex for access to members of the opposite sex (intrasexual selection). These two forms of selection mean that some individuals have greater reproductive success than others within a population, for example because they are more attractive or prefer more attractive partners to produce offspring. Successful males benefit from frequent mating and monopolizing access to one or more fertile females. Females can maximise the return on the energy they invest in reproduction by selecting and mating with the best males.
The concept was first articulated by Charles Darwin who wrote of a "second agency" other than natural selection, in which competition between mate candidates could lead to speciation. The theory was given a mathematical basis by Ronald Fisher in the early 20th century. Sexual selection can lead males to extreme efforts to demonstrate their fitness to be chosen by females, producing sexual dimorphism in secondary sexual characteristics, such as the ornate plumage of birds-of-paradise and peafowl, or the antlers of deer. Depending on the species, these rules can be reversed. This is caused by a positive feedback mechanism known as a Fisherian runaway, where the passing-on of the desire for a trait in one sex is as important as having the trait in the other sex in producing the runaway effect. Although the sexy son hypothesis indicates that females would prefer male offspring, Fisher's principle explains why the sex ratio is most often 1:1. Sexual selection is widely distributed in the animal kingdom, and is also found in plants and fungi.
These views were to some extent opposed by Alfred Russel Wallace, mostly after Darwin's death. He accepted that sexual selection could occur, but argued that it was a relatively weak form of selection. He argued that male–male competitions were forms of natural selection, but that the "drab" peahen's coloration is itself adaptive as camouflage. In his opinion, ascribing mate choice to females was attributing the ability to judge standards of beauty to animals (such as ) far too cognitively undeveloped to be capable of aesthetic feeling.
Darwin's ideas on sexual selection were met with scepticism by his contemporaries and not considered of great importance, until in the 1930s biologists decided to include sexual selection as a mode of natural selection.
Only in the 21st century have they become more important in biology; the theory is now seen as generally applicable and analogous to natural selection. A ten-year study, experimentally varying sexual selection on with other factors held constant, showed that sexual selection protected even an Inbreeding population against extinction. Population benefits of sexual selection explain the existence of males phys.org May 18, 2015 Report on a study by the University of East Anglia
This causes a dramatic increase in both the male's conspicuous feature and in female preference for it, resulting in marked sexual dimorphism, until practical physical constraints halt further exaggeration. A positive feedback loop is created, producing extravagant physical structures in the non-limiting sex. A classic example of female choice and potential runaway selection is the long-tailed widowbird. While males have long tails that are selected for by female choice, female tastes in tail length are still more extreme with females being attracted to tails longer than those that naturally occur.
Fisher understood that female preference for long tails may be passed on genetically, in conjunction with genes for the long tail itself. Long-tailed widowbird offspring of both sexes inherit both sets of genes, with females Gene expression their genetic preference for long tails, and males showing off the coveted long tail itself.Richard Dawkins presents a non-mathematical explanation of the runaway sexual selection process in his book The Blind Watchmaker.
Females that prefer long tailed males tend to have mothers that chose long-tailed fathers. As a result, they carry both sets of genes in their bodies. That is, genes for long tails and for preferring long tails become linked. The taste for long tails and tail length itself may therefore become correlated, tending to increase together. The more tails lengthen, the more long tails are desired. Any slight initial imbalance between taste and tails may set off an explosion in tail lengths. Fisher wrote that:The female widowbird chooses to mate with the most attractive long-tailed male so that her progeny, if male, will themselves be attractive to females of the next generation—thereby fathering many offspring that carry the female's genes. Since the rate of change in preference is proportional to the average taste amongst females, and as females desire to secure the services of the most sexually attractive males, an additive effect is created that, if unchecked, can yield exponential increases in a given taste and in the corresponding desired sexual attribute.
Since Fisher's initial conceptual model of the 'runaway' process, Russell Lande and Peter O'Donald have provided detailed mathematical proofs that define the circumstances under which runaway sexual selection can take place.
Alongside this, biologists have extended Darwin's formulation; Malte Andersson's widely accepted 1994 definition is that "sexual selection is the differences in reproduction that arise from variation among individuals in traits that affect success in competition over mates and fertilizations". Despite some practical challenges for biologists, the concept of sexual selection is "straightforward".
Sexual selection may sometimes generate features that help cause a species' extinction, as has historically been suggested for the giant antlers of the Irish elk ( Megaloceros giganteus) that became extinct in Holocene Eurasia (although climate-induced habitat deterioration and anthropogenic pressure are now considered more likely causes). It may, however, also do the opposite, driving species divergence—sometimes through elaborate changes in sex organs—such that new species emerge.Hosken, David J.; Stockley, Paula. " Sexual selection and genital evolution ." Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19.2 (2004): 87–93.Arnqvist, Göran. " Comparative evidence for the evolution of genitalia by sexual selection ." Nature 393.6687 (1998): 784. Sexual selection often interacts with natural selection to drive speciation.Maan, M.E.; Seehausen, O. "Ecology, sexual selection and speciation". Ecology Letters, 2011 Jun; 14(6). pp. 591-602. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01606.x. PMID 21375683.
From this point, several key studies were conducted on populations where females sought males, following Darwin’s description of the bird Barred buttonquail(Turnix suscitator). This included studies of species that consistently subverted expected sex hierarchy norms, such as studies of the Pipefish family(Syngnathinae) or Seahorse family(Hippocampus). Females of these species are generally larger, more colorful, and more aggressive than males. Most studies that succeeded after Darwin’s notes focused on this supposed reversal of animal behavior and attempted to understand what caused this female dominance. Most studies during the first half of the 20th century believed it to be a result of unbalanced sex ratios, i.e, many more females than males of a population, and for this hierarchy to be unchanging. Keynote studies on reproduction of fruit flies(Drosophila) in the 1960s, however, illustrated sexual diversity based on environmental factors such as food availability as well as sex ratios. What was also distinct about the studies was that they demonstrated how quickly the sexes' passive and dominant roles could change given the ecological conditions.
The redefinition and wide use of SRR as a tool in animal behavior came from the 1970s. Darwin’s mention of sex role reversal resulting from a much larger ratio of females to males was later picked up by researchers Stephen T. Emlen and Lewis W. Oring, who reworked the initial concept to a concrete definition. As opposed to females, SRR became redefined by males taking on the bulk of parental investment of offspring. Researchers were attempting to exclude human-biased projections onto animal behavior. SRR showing parental investment as opposed to “masculine” sexual behaviors performed by females was an attempt to exclude human bias from animal observation. The highest consistency of males taking on higher parental investment than females was most noted in fish, bird, or amphibian species. Biologists’ new understanding of sexual selection came from observing mate selection based upon resource availability. Resource and mating trade-offs exist for any sexually reproducing organism. For example, male Sandpipers(Actitis macularia) are generally responsible for caring for the nest and protecting eggs, a task seen in other bird species to be shared with or exclusive to female members. Thus, other mating opportunities for male sandpipers are a tradeoff for offspring care, making them unavailable for breeding. Generally, the sex that has to produce eggs and care for offspring in the zygote stage will divest resources from itself in the post-zygotic stage. This means that during this period, they would be unable to engage in other mating opportunities, providing a barrier to mating rates. Instances of the zygote carrying sex (i.e, the female) devoting less time to post-zygote parenting do present many questions to population behaviour.
After Emlen and Oring's publications, questions remained on what environmental conditions led to SRR. A smaller ratio of males to females is the most widely accepted reason for SRR. Populations that consistently have a higher number of females to males have been noted for distinct dimorphism, as well as males undertaking higher parental investment in particular species. Animals exhibiting monogamous mating behaviour very rarely have traits of SRR. The most common mating type to take on SSR is polygamous species, in which dominant individuals have access to many mating partners, and conspecifics are driven away. Examples of polygamy coinciding with SRR are consistent with the Gulf pipefish(Syngnathus scovelli). Pipefish have been studied for decades due to observations of males being the choosier sex of mating partners, and females selected based on markers of higher quality, such as sex and the presence of secondary traits. Dominant females had numerous male mates, while other females were driven away. The study found that the same line of strong sexual dimorphism in polygamous species was carried to SRR examples, with females being the ones facing sexual selection as opposed to males.
Other factors, like resource availability, may affect populations switching towards or away from SRR. Recently, however, biologists studying numerous different insect species have found that nest availability could be an enormous factor. The relative availability of nests for females to lay eggs was a determining factor in whether or not parental investment by males and female sexual dominance became present in the Broad-winged katydid(Microcentrum rhombifolium). When nests are less frequent in an environment, females undergo intrasexual competition to gain access to males, and males generally take on more parenting responsibilities. Co-parenting becomes much more common for populations of katydids in a habitat that allows for more nests, and female aggression decreases.
Researchers now understand that the dynamics between male and female counterparts are more complex than “female dominant” and “male dominant.” The field is studying the adaptation of different sexual roles as a result of fluid factors such as sex ratio or resource investment.
Among the many instances of sexual selection in mammals is extreme sexual dimorphism, with males as much as six times heavier than females, and male fighting for dominance among . Dominant males establish large harems of several dozen females; unsuccessful males may attempt to copulate with a harem male's females if the dominant male is inattentive. This forces the harem male to defend his territory continuously, not feeding for as much as three months.
Also seen in mammals is sex-role reversal, as in the highly social , where a large female is dominant within a pack, and female–female competition is observed. The dominant female produces most of the offspring; the subordinate females are nonbreeding, providing altruistic care to the young.
Many forms of sexual selection exist among the insects. Parental care is often provided by female insects, as in bees, but male parental care is found in Belostomatidae water bugs, where the male, after fertilizing the eggs, allows the female to glue her eggs onto his back. He broods them until the nymphs hatch 2–4 weeks later. The eggs are large and reduce the ability of the male to fertilise other females and catch prey, and increases its predation risk.
Among the Firefly (Lampyrid beetles), males fly in darkness and emit a species-specific pattern of light flashes, which are answered by perching receptive females. The colour and temporal variation of the flashes contribute to success in attracting females. Among the , sexual selection is common. In the mealworm beetle, Tenebrio molitor, males release pheromones to attract females to mate. Females choose mates based on whether they are infected, and on their mass.
Many different tactics are used by snakes to acquire mates. Ritual combat between males for the females they want to mating with includes topping, a behaviour exhibited by most Viperidae, in which one male twists around the vertically elevated fore body of its opponent and forcing it downward. Neck biting is common while the snakes are entwined.
Many bird species make use of , the females preferring males with songs that are complex and varied in amplitude, structure, and frequency. Larger males have deeper songs and increased mating success.
Fungi appear to make use of sexual selection, although they also often reproduce asexually. In the Basidiomycetes, the sex ratio is biased towards males, implying sexual selection there. Male–male competition to fertilise occurs in fungi including yeasts. Pheromone signaling is used by female gametes and by conidia, implying male choice in these cases. Female–female competition may also occur, indicated by the much faster evolution of female-biased genes in fungi.
|
|